By: News Archive
From scissors and staplers to car keys and cell phones, we pass objects to other people every day. We often try to pass the objects so that the handle or other useful feature is facing the appropriate direction for the person receiving the item, but new research involving the University of East Anglia shows that we are less accommodating when it comes to handing over our own belongings.
Picking up objects is such a routine part of everyday life that we don’t often think about how we do it, but the research shows that our actions often contain a prosocial element. When we pick up a mug, for example, we typically pick it up by the handle because that is most comfortable. But when we hand the mug to someone else, we might turn it so that the handle faces the person receiving it.
The findings are published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
“The associations or attachments that we have with an object leak into our movements in unintended ways when we interact with them,” says psychology researcher and lead study author Merryn Constable of the University of Toronto. “The act of facilitating another person’s action is somewhat inhibited when the object that we’re passing is something that we own, but the effects are so subtle that they are likely to go unnoticed.”
Constable and colleagues, including Dr Andrew Bayliss of UEA's School of Psychology, wanted to find out whether specific social factors, such as ownership, might influence this behavior – that is, are we just as helpful when passing our own mug as we are when passing someone else’s?
In two experiments, the researchers examined passing behavior among 42 pairs of friends. A week or two before the actual experiment, each participant received a mug to keep; the mugs varied only in their background color. The participants were told to use their mug every day, at home or at work, and to make sure that only they used it. This instruction was given to ensure that the participants would feel ownership over the mug.
For the experiment, the friends sat across from each other at a table and the experimenter placed a mug in a specific location on the table. One participant, designated the “passer,” was told to pick up the mug and place it in front of his or her friend in a natural manner. In some cases, the friend receiving the mug was told to pick it up by the handle; in other cases, the friend was instructed to remain still.
The person doing the passing and the mug that was being passed both varied randomly from trial to trial. The researchers tracked the location of each participant’s hand and the location of the mug using a motion-capture system.
In line with previous research, people passed the mug slightly differently depending on whether the friend was going to pick it up afterward – that is, passers rotated the handle closer to the friend’s hand when they expected him or her to grasp the mug.
Interestingly, the researchers found that passers rotated the handle slightly less when handing over their own mug compared to when they handed over someone else’s mug. This less helpful behavior occurred both when they passed their friend’s mug and when they passed a mug belonging to the researcher, a relative stranger.
The findings from these two studies indicate that passers seemed to help less when passing their own mug to their friend rather than helping more when passing the friend’s own mug, which surprised the researchers:
“We were expecting that the effect would be related to helping more if the object that is being passed is owned by the receiver,” says Constable. “It’s possible the prosocial behavior demonstrated by this group of participants was influenced by their self-interest concerning possessions.”
Overall, the two experiments underscore the importance of paying attention to the social context of our physical interactions:
“These findings reveals how the subtleties of our social world can play out in how we interact physically with objects and people,” Constable concludes.
Co-authors on the research include Andrew P. Bayliss (University of East Anglia), Steven P. Tipper (University of York), Ana P. Spaniol (Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte), and Jay Pratt and Timothy N. Welsh (both of the University of Toronto).
Nurses around the world use intuition to work out how sick a patient is before triaging for treatment according to new research from the University of East Anglia.
Read moreOn Sunday 24 September, University of East Anglia (UEA) nursing apprentice Francessca Turrell will be taking part in a charity skydive for Alzheimers Society, a UK care and research charity for people with dementia and their carers.
Read moreLogo Rewind: Trademarks of Medieval Norwich is a new book from UEA Publishing Project, in collaboration with CreativeUEA and featuring the work of Darren Leader, which will focus on the stories of Norwichs medieval merchants marks found in different locat
Read more